TY - JOUR
T1 - Processes of understanding anaphora
AU - Murphy, Gregory L.
N1 - Funding Information:
This research was supported by Grant MH-20021 to Herbert Clark and by a Bio-Medical Research Grant from Brown University. The author gratefully acknowledges the helpful comments of Hiram Brownell, Peter Eimas, Barbara Malt, and Ivan Sag on earlier drafts of this article. Frank Halasz and Carolyn Foss deserve thanks for their development of the computer system on which the experiments were run. It is a pleasure to acknowledge the valuable advice of Herbert Clark during all phases of the research and writing of this article. Requests for reprints should be addressed to Gregory L. Murphy, Hunter Laboratory of Psychology. Brown University, Providence, R.I. 02912.
Copyright:
Copyright 2014 Elsevier B.V., All rights reserved.
PY - 1985/6
Y1 - 1985/6
N2 - Three experiments explored the understanding of anaphoric verb phrases, verb phrases with ellipsis or a substitute element that must be interpreted through reference to an antecedent. For example, Mom tried out the new car after Dad did contains the pro-verb did. Linguistic analysis of anaphoric verb phrases (VPs) suggests that people use two understanding strategies, one accessing a surface representation of the antecedent in short-term memory and one based on plausible reasoning in a mental "discourse model" of the text. Three experiments investigated how these strategies are coordinated in understanding. Subjects read brief stories one sentence at a time, and their reading times were recorded. The results found that a number of factors affected these reading times. First, long antecedents and antecedents that were syntactically "inconsistent" slowed comprehension. Second, both of these effects held primarily when the antecedent was in the preceding sentence; placing the antecedent further away also slowed reading times overall. Third, the experiments found no reliable differences between two different categories of anaphora ("deep" and "surface" anaphora), nor did any of the other effects depend on the type of anaphor. These results suggest constraints on a theory of how readers coordinate syntactic and pragmatic processes used in interpreting anaphoric verb phrases.
AB - Three experiments explored the understanding of anaphoric verb phrases, verb phrases with ellipsis or a substitute element that must be interpreted through reference to an antecedent. For example, Mom tried out the new car after Dad did contains the pro-verb did. Linguistic analysis of anaphoric verb phrases (VPs) suggests that people use two understanding strategies, one accessing a surface representation of the antecedent in short-term memory and one based on plausible reasoning in a mental "discourse model" of the text. Three experiments investigated how these strategies are coordinated in understanding. Subjects read brief stories one sentence at a time, and their reading times were recorded. The results found that a number of factors affected these reading times. First, long antecedents and antecedents that were syntactically "inconsistent" slowed comprehension. Second, both of these effects held primarily when the antecedent was in the preceding sentence; placing the antecedent further away also slowed reading times overall. Third, the experiments found no reliable differences between two different categories of anaphora ("deep" and "surface" anaphora), nor did any of the other effects depend on the type of anaphor. These results suggest constraints on a theory of how readers coordinate syntactic and pragmatic processes used in interpreting anaphoric verb phrases.
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=0000472512&partnerID=8YFLogxK
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=0000472512&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1016/0749-596X(85)90029-4
DO - 10.1016/0749-596X(85)90029-4
M3 - Article
AN - SCOPUS:0000472512
SN - 0749-596X
VL - 24
SP - 290
EP - 303
JO - Journal of Memory and Language
JF - Journal of Memory and Language
IS - 3
ER -